Home Blog Page 19

From the archives: What is Hypnosis?

0

This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 4, Issue 1, from 1990.

Take statements like these: ‘I don’t remember where I heard that’; ‘I’ve forgotten the word’; ‘I don’t know why I did that’; ‘It doesn’t hurt’; ‘I’ve gone deaf’; ‘I can’t see it’; ‘It’s red’. There are two ways of finding out whether these statements are true. One is to simply ask the speaker, and believe whatever he says. The other is to sit down and figure out independent ways of getting at the truth.

Hypnosis is a case in point, and for my present purpose I intend to deliberately restrict my main references to one of the latest and largest offerings on the subject – Hypnosis: the Cognitive-Behavioural Perspective, edited by Nicholas P. Spanos and John F. Chaves (Prometheus Books 1989). This is because the book is available to the general public, and saves me from appending a list of references to professional journals that you won’t have ready access to, and won’t consult anyway. The book contains 19 papers in all, and the text is backed up by about 1,500 references that you can chase up if you want to.

What is hypnosis? Spanos and Chaves point out that, for most laypeople and many research workers and health care providers, it involves a trance, or at least an altered state of consciousness. It is brought on by some repetitive verbal rituals, known as an induction procedure. The person hypnotised becomes a passive automaton, and comes under the control of the hypnotist.

The vogue began with the German physician Anton Mesmer in the late 18th century. It became entwined with a range of other occult beliefs, and took on a new lease of life. And now here we are towards the end of the 20th century, with the American Psychological Association sheltering a fully-fledged Division of Psychological Hypnosis (are there other kinds?).

Book cover of 'Hypnosis: the cognitive-behavioral perspective', highlighting the H and S in 'Hypnosis', above a single eye and a spread of four fingers at the lower end of the image.
Cover of ‘Hypnosis: The Cognitive-Behavioral Perspective – A Reference Guide to Contemporary Research’, edited by Nicholas P. Spanos and John F. Chaves

It is about 40 years now since T. R. Sarbin (one of this book’s contributors) threw out the long-held belief in an ‘altered state of consciousness.’ And in the 1960s, T.X. Barber finally unsheathed the simple weapon that has been the death of so many groundless belief systems – the control group.

Usually one group was given a hypnotic induction procedure. A second group was asked to just imagine whatever was suggested to them. And a third group was simply urged to do their best to respond to suggestions.

‘Hypnotised’ subjects responded to suggestions for age regression, hallucination, amnesia, pain reduction, and so forth – but so did the controls. And those who were simply told, “Do your best” did just as well as those who were ‘hypnotised.’ (I’m afraid the word ‘hypnotised’ often has to go about with quotes acting as bodyguards.)

Other workers soon found that non-hypnotic subjects also did as well in producing “so called immoral, self-destructive, or criminal behaviour.” It turned out that all subjects knew perfectly well that they must be safe from harm, since they were aware they were taking part in experiments in an academic setting.

The subjects who showed up best in tests of hypnotic susceptibility were those who had been asked to pretend to be hypnotised. And hypnotic performance could be noticeably improved by some training. In other words, what had been thought of as a genetically endowed susceptibility was in fact a skill that could be learned.

The fact is that hypnotic subjects know how hypnotic subjects are supposed to behave, and their general goal is “to behave like a hypnotised person as this is continuously defined by the operator and understood by the subject.” ( R.W. White) For many subjects, merely defining the situation as hypnosis results in them classifying everyday behaviours like arm raising as ‘involuntary.’

And T.X. Barber and D.S. Calverley made the amusing discovery that when a group of subjects were told that hypnosis was a test of gullibility, “hypnotic responding was virtually nullified.”

When a subject is told that he will perform a certain action on cue, after ‘waking up’, is that action really beyond his control? Thirteen subjects were told to scratch their ear when they heard the word ‘psychology’, and they all did so. The hypnotist gave the impression that the experiment was over, and had an informal conversation with a colleague, in which the cue word was used. 9 of the subjects failed to respond. When the hypnotist then intimated that the experiment was still in progress, 7 of those 9 began responding again. Another experimenter found that all post-hypnotic responding stopped when he left the room, apparently to attend an emergency.

In a similar experiment, Spanos and his associates found that subjects all dutifully coughed when they heard the word ‘psychology’ in the experimental situation. But Spanos had arranged for a confederate to pose as a lost student asking for the psychology department. None of the subjects responded to the cue word.

I.F. Hoyt and J. F. Kihlstrom have concluded that “post-hypnotic information processing is no different than non-hypnotic information processing.” Subjects are sometimes given a post-hypnotic suggestion that they will not remember certain key words. Do they really forget these words? According to their verbal reports, they do. But according to their galvanic skin resistance, they don’t. In another example, subjects are given a list of words to learn. They are then ‘hypnotised’, and given another list to learn. This second list has been constructed so as to interfere with the recall of the first list. Some subjects are then given a post-hypnotic amnesia suggestion to forget the second list. Other subjects are given no suggestion. When recall is tested later, subjects in both groups recall the first list at the same level. William C. Coe asks simply: “Is their amnesia credible?”

Spanos and others found that between 40 and 63 per cent of their ‘amnesic’ subjects later admitted that they had suppressed their reports. Coe wonders: “Perhaps we should wonder how many did not confess?” And he comments: “The ‘skill’ they employ is not reporting.” It is probably no surprise to learn that simulators are just as successful in employing this ‘skill.’ Furthermore, most amnesiacs will confess to remembering more and more of the ‘forgotten’ material under adequate pressure, “to the extent that they have nothing left to remember when amnesia is lifted.”

Hypnotic deafness? If you get someone to read or speak into a microphone, and feed back the sound of his voice into headphones after a momentary time lag, his speech will become seriously disrupted, with slurring, hesitations, and stammering. Hypnotic subjects claiming to be deaf show the same disruptions. ‘High-susceptible’ subjects and ‘low-susceptible’ subjects have been told that they are deaf in one ear. Then pairs of words have been presented simultaneously, one member of the pair to each ear. Subjects should only be able to hear words presented to their ‘good’ ear. In fact both groups show the same number of intrusions from the ‘deaf’ ear.

Colour blindness? When hypnotically colour-blind subjects are shown the Isihara ‘malingering’ card, they report that they can’t see the number that in fact can be seen by all genuinely red-green blind individuals.

Post-hypnotic negative hallucinations? Hypnotically blind subjects continue to process the visual information they claim not to see.

There are a number of standard ways of reducing the effects of pain (eg self-distraction, placebos, relaxation, cognitive re-interpretation, positive imagery). Does hypnosis do a better job? Perhaps the best-known (and most often quoted) person to use mesmerism for surgical pain was the 19th-century physician John Esdaile. He reported thousands of minor surgical procedures as well as several hundred major surgical procedures. Medical workers in Austria, in France, and in the United States all tried to replicate his successes. They all failed. Like the acupuncture miracle-workers of China, Esdaile plied his trade in a distant clime (in his case, India) – “far from the din of skeptical colleagues” in the polite phrase of John F. Chaves.

When the time came to investigate Esdaile’s achievements, the Bengal government appointed a commission. Esdaile selected only ten patients for observation. Three were discarded because they appeared to be unresponsive to his techniques. One case was “inconclusive”.Three showed “convulsive movements of the upper limbs, writhing of the body, distortion of the features, giving the face a hideous expression of suppressed agony…” The remaining three showed no outward sign of pain, though two of them showed erratic pulse rates. This was hardly the wonder anesthetic that everyone had been led to expect.

Esdaile’s tiny (and selective) sample did not take into account the wide variation in different people’s ability to tolerate pain. More recent attempts to use hypnotic analgesia have suffered the same flaws that have ruined the claims for acupuncture analgesia: the treatment has almost always been accompanied by chemical anesthetics, sedation, or local anesthesia. In fact suggestions for reducing the perception of pain can be effective whether accompanied by hypnotic induction or not. And sadly, as Chaves points out, “a recent review of significant developments in medical hypnosis over the past 25 years fails to cite a single report of hypno-analgesia…”

Joyce L. D’Eon reports that in childbirth, “hypnotic procedures have failed to meet the grandiose claims that have sometimes been made for them.” Without the counter-check of a control group, it is all too easy to attribute an easy birth to the use of hypnosis, because “anywhere from 9 to 24 per cent of women experience relatively painless childbirth without any intervention.”

Richard F. Q. Johnson asked 42 prominent researchers if they had ever tried to produce blisters by hypnotic suggestion. Seven said they had obtained positive results. But none had published their results, and they were very skeptical of their findings. They suspected that highly motivated subjects might secretly injure themselves to produce the results the hypnotist wanted.

Nevertheless, further research is called for, especially comparing normal subjects with those who have sensitive skin. After all, anxiety is associated with the production and intensity of other skin ailments, such as hives. Nearly all cases of religious stigmata can be explained in terms of deliberate self-injury. The bleedings are brought to the attention of the investigator only after they have begun, and it is almost impossible to keep a 24-hour watch on the individuals.

Warts? If they are left untreated, they will generally go away of their own accord after two or three years. In a controlled study, 17 patients with warts on both sides of the body were given hypnotic induction, then told that the warts would disappear from one side of the body. Some warts did in fact go away – from both sides of the body. But any treatment that the patient believes in is likely to produce results just as dramatic as those claimed for hypnosis. Johnson summarises: “the skin may at times be strongly influenced by thinking and suggestion. Nevertheless, the precise relationship between verbal suggestion and changes in the skin has yet to be determined.”

Henderikus J. Stam reports: “The use of hypnosis for the treatment of cancer pain, like other psychological techniques for the treatment of this problem, has remained largely untested. The bulk of this literature is in the form of case reports.” His conclusions offer little hope: “Where does this leave the literature on the treatment of cancer pain? More or less where it began, unfortunately. The lack of systematic studies and the continued exaggerated claims made for this technique have left it in scientific and therapeutic limbo.”

Where does the law stand in all this? In the case of K. Bianchi, the so-called Hillside Strangler, the Los Angeles courts gave a curious solidity to the spirit form known as hypnosis. The law ruled that testimony from hypnotised witnesses was not admissible in court. As H. P. de Groot and M.I. Gwynn conclude in their discussion of the case, “it makes little sense to ask whether or not Bianchi was ‘really hypnotised,’ because the construct ‘hypnosis’ has little utility as a scientific account of hypnotic responding.”

In Canadian law, hypnotic suggestion, along with such influences as drugs and alcohol, is allowed as a basis for the defence of ‘automatism’. And the American Law Institute’s Model Penal Code claims that anyone following  hypnotic suggestion is not acting voluntarily, so he can’t be considered criminally liable. But various American states offer differing viewpoints.

In England, a judge in Maidstone has ruled that the testimony of four witnesses was not admissible in court, because they had been previously ‘hypnotised’. By contrast, the cognitive-behavioural point of view takes the position that the actions of a ‘hypnotised’ subject are voluntary. T.X. Barber has made the point that explaining a hypnotic subject’s behaviour in terms of a trance or altered state of consciousness is like explaining a shaman’s behaviour in terms of spirit possession. As for hypnotising witnesses to get at the facts, ‘there is no conclusive evidence, either anecdotal or experimental, to indicate that hypnosis can act as a “truth serum.”‘

There is not even any known method for detecting whether anyone is simulating ‘hypnosis’ or not. The likeliest result of allowing the police to use hypnotists would be “the confident reporting of inaccurate information” (P. W. Sheehan and J . Tilden). All in all, “the kinds of experiences and behaviours that are elicited by hypnotic procedures can also be produced by placebos and other expectancy-modification procedures.”

As long ago as 1962, T. R. Sarbin proposed that the term ‘hypnosis’ should be stricken from the professional vocabulary of psychology. That day has still not come, although the cognitive skills involved are being more often referred to nowadays by such terms as ‘goal-directed fantasy’ or ‘think-with suggestions.’

Hypnosis was born at a time when theological explanations were just beginning to give way to the rationality rules of science. So in the late 20th century, who is it who’s still hanging on to this concept of human conduct as a function of strange internal forces, and a vocabulary more suited to occult mysteries? The answer turns out to be – the clinicians. William C. Coe’s study of hypnosis journals is revealing. “It seems that the vast majority of clinicians prefer using special state concepts in vague ways, perhaps naively, or perhaps to mystify purposely. It seems equally clear that the vast majority of experimental investigators avoid using special state concepts.”

From a scientific viewpoint, “hypnotic induction rituals are viewed as historical curiosities that reflect outmoded 19th-century attempts to conceptualise the behaviours associated with this topic as linked in some way to sleep.”

“In short,” say Spanos and Chaves, “clinical hypnosis as a research area appears to be at roughly the same point as experimental hypnosis research before Barber began his systematic controlled experimentation in the late 1950s.”

São Paulo government boasts about how much money it wastes on alternative medicines

This story was originally written in Portuguese, and published to the website of Revista Questão de Ciência. It appears here with permission.

In May, the city government of São Paulo in Brazil published an advertorial to the website of Rio de Janeiro newspaper O Globo, boasting about offering, in the municipal health system, many of the 29 integrative and complementary practices (PICs) approved by the Ministry of Health for use in Brazil’s Unified Health System (SUS). To be clear, this was “paid content” (or, in the current euphemism, “branded content”), published not because the newspaper editor thinks it is important, but because someone bought the space.

We can leave it to political analysts to figure out why São Paulo taxpayers’ money is being used to buy online space in a Rio de Janeiro newspaper. The issue here, after all, is science. The acronym “PICs” serves as an umbrella for a series of therapeutic proposals and medications that have one thing in common: either their risks and benefits have never been properly evaluated by science, or they have been evaluated and ultimately rejected.

The provision of PICs by the government is unethical and uneconomical, an argument that we have made extensively across a series of previous publications in Revista Questão de Ciência (some of which can be found here , here and here ).

The examples cited in São Paulo city government’s advertorial include herbal remedies such as valerian, for which there is some evidence of efficacy, albeit weak and inconclusive, plus some which do not even have a completed clinical trial, such as espinheira-santa, as well as low-impact physical activity modalities, such as yoga and tai-chi, from which it is reasonable to expect some benefit, even if nonspecific. It goes on to cover absurd and unscientific practices, such as auriculotherapy, and others that are clearly placebos, such as acupuncture.

A person with acupuncture needles protruding from their hand, and manicured nails out of focus, rests their hand on a cloth-covered surface in a treatment room. View from midway down the forearm.
Hand acupuncture. Via RDNE Stock Project, Pexels

The paid article is exemplary in the sense that it exercises several of the fallacies and contradictions normally used to justify the unjustifiable – the provision of unfounded or dubious therapies by the public service. One of them is the contrast between “treating the symptom” (which is supposedly what science-based medicine does) and “treating the patient as a whole” (supposedly the province of PICs).

This is obviously false; conventional medicine also requires a holistic view. Where it falls short of this goal, it is a flaw that needs to be corrected – it is not reason to turn to nonsense. As British doctor, journalist and researcher Ben Goldacre has pointed out, just because airplanes are uncomfortable and sometimes crash, we are not better off turning to flying carpets.

In this respect, PICs end up acting as an accidental pretext for maintaining the industrial, cold and impersonal nature of typical medical care in the public system and in private health plans: to continue the aeronautical metaphor, instead of improving the seats and the quality of the on-board service, we are offered flying carpets, which may be beautiful and soft but, at the end of the day, go nowhere.

Satisfied customer fallacy

This does not mean that the softness of the carpet does not provide some momentary comfort, and that the beautiful patterns of the fabric do not help the patient to distract themselves and forget that they have not moved. The problems are the lack of objective change – the trip did not happen, and the real illness was not treated; there is intrinsic dishonesty in selling illusions.

Illusions of causality – when the mind ends up accepting as true cause and effect relationships that are, in reality, false – are favoured when there is high “cause density” and high “effect density”.

In the case of health issues, a high effect density means that the disease in question is either chronic – the symptoms come and go – or has a high rate of spontaneous remission. In other words, there are diseases that tend to resolve themselves, or with symptoms that go through cycles of relief. This is why any cold remedy “works,” whether it’s chicken soup, a multivitamin pill, or homeopathy: the cold was going to go away anyway.

High “cause density,” in turn, occurs when the number of people trying the pseudo-cure is high, which increases the chance of “positive results” arising by mere chance. It is overly optimistic to imagine that this mechanism is unsustainable in the long term – that random cures cannot sustain the popularity of an ineffective (or even ineffective and highly dangerous) treatment for years or even centuries. One need only recall the example of bloodletting, popular for three millennia.

The two densities appear very clearly in the publicity material of the São Paulo city government published in O Globo – both, of course, falsely interpreted as real evidence of effectiveness. The text cites huge numbers of attendance:

Auriculotherapy (derived from acupuncture which consists of pressing nerve points on the ear) and acupuncture are the main individual modalities performed. There were, respectively, 192,403 sessions (equivalent to 51% of all services) and 45,417 procedures in the first half of 2023.

And, a few paragraphs later:

From January to November 16, 2023, the municipal administration delivered 13,996,700 herbal medicines to pharmacies in the municipal health network, an increase of 29.78% compared to the same period in 2022. In the last nine years, more than 60 million units were released.

These data are interspersed with testimonials from four satisfied users, two of whom have chronic conditions. Given the extremely high cause density and prevalence of chronic cases, and therefore high effect density, it would probably have been possible to find hundreds or even thousands of positive testimonials. But the same can be said of chloroquine or ivermectin for Covid-19, and for the same reasons: a huge number of patients treated and a condition that, in most cases, tends to resolve itself.

Using the glow of popularity to overshadow the lack of concrete evidence of effectiveness is a common rhetorical strategy in the PIC universe.

How much does it cost?

One piece of information that is notable for its absence from the press release is the amount of spending (or “investment”) involved in offering “more than one million alternative procedures” in “471 Basic Health Units.” We read that the “city government invests in training and updating professionals in this area”, but we are not told how much is invested.

As a matter of principle, any penny of public health funds spent on procedures and therapies of zero or highly dubious effectiveness is a waste; as a practical matter, in times of fiscal tightening and limited funding, any waste is irresponsible.

Direct financial loss, however, is just one of the problems brought about by the endorsement of alternative therapies by the government. Added to this are the very real dangers to citizens’ health and a profound miseducation effect.

The danger exists, either because the risk profile of the practice offered is unknown, since most PICs have never been adequately tested for safety or efficacy, or because the illusion of the flying carpet can mask serious health conditions, or lead the patient to neglect real treatments. The miseducation comes from the false equivalence proposed between scientific knowledge and mythologies such as vital energy and water memory; between Aeronautical Engineering and fairytale magic.

This is sabotage against the most fundamental scientific literacy of the population.

The UK riots showed why we have to dismantle the far-right’s misinformation machines

On 29th July, a knifeman entered a Taylor Swift-themed dance class in Southport, 20 miles from where we are right now, and stabbed nine children and two adults. Three of the girls died – six-year-old Bebe King, seven-year-old Elsie Dot Stancombe, and nine-year-old Alice da Silva Aguiar. A community and a country was in shock. This doesn’t happen in the UK. People were angry, and they were scared, and they were sad. They planned a vigil to the memory of those three girls, in Southport, on July 30th.

Initially, information on the perpetrator was scarce. In the vacuum, rumours circulated. Channel 3 Now News released the name of the murderer – Ali Al-Shakati, a young Muslim who had recently arrived by boat. This was soon amplified far and wide, initially on Telegram, starting with a 55-year-old ‘social commentator’ who had appeared multiple times on GB News and Talk TV, called Bernie Spofforth. Bernie, aka Artemisfornow, is an influential conspiracy theorist, who has grown a following for her denial of the pandemic, her warnings over vaccine harms, her scaremongering about 15-minute cities, and all the usual stuff. At one point, she had 250k followers on Twitter.

The killer’s name was soon in every Telegram chat I’ve been following since the White Rose investigation. It came up multiple times in Tommy Robinson’s channel, the far-right former leader of the EDL, who grew a mammoth Telegram following during the pandemic, after pivoting to Covid conspiracism. It made it to Twitter, where people like Andrew Tate, Lawrence Fox, Tommy Robinson, Isabelle Oakenshot, and Darren Grimes started speculating about what had happened and who was to blame. Was the month-old Labour government too soft on immigration? Is this further proof we need to stop the boats? Had we been too accepting to people from other places? Were we undergoing an invasion, and was this the first sign?

A screenshot from a Telegram group channel showing photos of riot vans and a police officer with a man who has a full beard. The text reads "SOUTHPORT An Arab man was found with a balaclava and machete at the vigil for the 10 girls in Southport. This is what triggered the scenes down St Lukes road, don't let the media twist It"

The message has been forwarded from another source.
A message in the Telegram group “Liverpools Peoples Resistance” claims the Southport riot was triggered by the arrest of an “Arab man” with a balaclava and machete.

Nigel Farage, the new MP for Clacton, had questions, so he took to Twitter to ask them, pointing out that the police hadn’t said anything about the murderer, so what aren’t we being told? What are they hiding?

People were angry, and now they had a name, a group to blame and a venue. They descended on the Southport vigil, some wearing hoods, many carrying cans of alcohol, many from outside of the region but plenty from within it. If things were struggling to remain peaceful, the next bit of news lit the tinderbox: spreading around Telegram and other online groups was an image of a man being arrested, apparently a Muslim with a machete who was stopped on his way to the vigil, intent on doing harm.

That was all it took, and from there carnage ensued. Vehicles were set alight, locals were attacked, the police – who had responded to the murder of three children just over 24 hours earlier – were pelted with bricks chipped out of walls and dug out of the very road itself. It became a riot, and it headed to Southport Mosque. Thankfully, nobody was killed.

Misinformation and propaganda

It was all based on lies. Ali Al-Shakati didn’t exist; the killer was called Axel Rudakubana. Rather than a recent arrival on a small boat, he was born and raised in Wales. Rather than a Muslim, he was Christian, born to strongly religious Rwandan parents. And rather than covering this up in a sinister fashion, the silence from the police was because he was 17, and therefore a minor, with a legal right to anonymity until a trial judge releases his name, which in the cases of minors usually only happens after sentencing.

So how had Channel 3 Now News, the original source of the name, got this so wrong? It’s because there is no Channel3Now – in the UK, the third TV channel is called ITV, we don’t have a Channel 3. The source called Channel3Now is a now-suspended YouTube channel, whose first videos were Russian language motor racing videos. The channel was bought, repurposed, renamed, and retooled to try to generate income by mass producing news articles, regardless of their veracity. It’s unclear who first sent it to Bernie Spofforth – though she has since been arrested, so we may find out its route to her in due course – but once it was with her the existing conspiracist infrastructure of the UK went into full force, flinging lies to every confused and angry person in the network.

A screenshot of a tweet from Jess Phillips which reads "Nigel Farage could yesterday have had the questions, he claims are unanswered, answered if he had bothered to turn up to parliament and ask them during the statement on the incidents in Southport. He didn't turn up, he grifted instead."

Farage, for his part, has since admitted that his source for his rumour mongering and questions to just-ask was… alleged sex trafficker and rapist Andrew Tate. These are the waters in which the Right Honourable Member for Clacton-on-Sea swims, apparently. As Labour MP Jess Phillips pointed out, if he truly had genuine questions about what was going on, Farage could have turned up for work and asked those questions of the people who knew, but instead he decided to put out an opportunistic video on Twitter. Far from apologising, Farage has only doubled down, releasing further videos warning that the worst is to come unless immigration is curbed.

And what of that Muslim with a machete, whose arrest further nudged the mob into violence? His name was Jordan Davies, and rather than being an immigrant, he was from Southport. Rather than a machete, it was a flick knife. And rather than being a Muslim, he was attending the riot as one of the far-right sympathisers, having posted to his Instagram: “Show these dirty little rat bastards who there fuckin dealing with. Gunna be hard to keep it peaceful not going to lie”. Again, he messaged that while carrying a knife.

The rumour about him being Muslim seems to have been based on him having a beard in a grainy photo of his arrest. The source of it is still being investigated, as presumably is their intent and how aware they were of his identity at the time they lied about him being a Muslim agitator.

For the complete avoidance of any doubt, there were no Muslims involved in this horrible crime – the killer wasn’t Muslim, and the people inciting the violence were not Muslim. But on Friday – apparently in response to the crime and in the memory of those girls – a mob gathered outside of the Abdullah Quilliam Society Mosque in Liverpool, whipped up on social media by all these same forces. Fortunately, they were hugely outnumbered, and the leader of the Mosque, Adam Kelwick, even came out to speak directly with some of the protesters, talking some down from their fears, listening to what they had to say, and hugging one of them.

We went to see Adam speaking at an anti-racism event, and he was a great speaker, calling for compassion and humanity, even for those who are yelling slurs and aggressive things at us, because everyone is a human being, and people are scared and confused. And he also made the absolutely correct point that the killer was not a Muslim, but even if he had been a Muslim, that in no way justifies a mob attacking a random mosque, any more than we should be burning down churches because the killer was Christian. This wasn’t justice, and it wasn’t even vengeance, it was a race riot. And it wasn’t the only one.

Riots and racism

Over the next few days, riots would break out at cities and towns across the UK. There were nazi salutes on the streets of Leicester, swastika tattoos on rioters who had travelled from Stoke to join the mob in Sunderland, and people going house to house in Middlesbrough, kicking in doors and stopping cars to try to find people who weren’t white. In Rotherham and in Tamworth, crowds gathered outside of hotels where asylum seekers were being kept, putting out the windows, daubing racist graffiti, and trying to set alight to the building to burn the families inside. All of this purportedly in the name of legitimate concerns, all of it denied as being influenced by the far right. The swastika tattoos were on people who had reasonable concerns. A hotel filled with families and kids was torched in the name of children’s safety.

Even the anti-racism demonstration we joined in Liverpool was convened in order to march down to the waterfront to oppose a rally that had been gathering there, where six or seven hundred people – mostly, but not all, men – had been gathering. We left before their protest turned violent, and before they fought with the police, and broke their way into a phone shop in town, one that happened to be owned by a Muslim. They eventually left the town centre, only to reconvene in Walton, supposedly to gather outside of a Mosque, before eventually setting fire to a community library.

Ostensibly, this was all meant to be a protest, but what they were protesting wasn’t clear. Some of the crowd in Liverpool and elsewhere were yelling “Who the fuck is Allah?” – none of this was to do with Muslims at all. They were also holding signs saying “Save Our Kids” and yelling about “nonces”, and again, this wasn’t a case of child sexual abuse. Nine children had been stabbed by a 17-year-old. In many of the riots, they were yelling “we want our country back”.  

The “Save Our Kids” banners, of course, give away some of the origins of this movement and how the confused crowd had been convened: it is a QAnon slogan, notionally about an epidemic of children being trafficked into sexual slavery by a shadowy cabal of elites. It also crosses into the anti-Muslim narrative around ‘grooming gangs’ – the far right talking point which takes the real-life instances of paedophile rings among some Muslim communities in some towns across the UK, and emphasises it in order to falsely insinuate that the majority of child abuse in the UK is committed by Muslims. The same might also explain the gentleman who had constructed a seven-foot tall Christian cross out of plywood and daubed it with slogans – because the same spaces that favour QAnon have also been amplifying Christian Nationalist imagery.

That QAnon slogans and Christian Nationalism had spilled out onto the streets of Liverpool ought to have been a shock, except this was not the first time we’ve seen it happen – in my first-ever editorial for The Skeptic magazine, back in September 2020, I highlighted how anti-vaccine marches that were organised on Telegram were seeing people take to Merseyside streets with QAnon banners. Those anti-vaccine marches went away, but the groups didn’t, and the influences on those groups didn’t – they continued to boil away, simmering in a soup of conspiracism, Satanic Panic, culture war provocateuring, and anti-woke virtue signalling. They bubbled their way through claims around people who died suddenly, through Andrew Bridgen and Aseem Malhotra telling them the vaccines were deadly, through warnings about 15-minute cities and central bank digital currencies and drag show story hours and transvestigations, and they were kept on the boil by cynical actors stoking the flames.

Those cynical actors are, in my opinion, particularly responsible for the cries of “We want our country back”. The rioters wanted to take ‘our’ country back from the immigrants, Muslims and people of colour (the rioters were not asking about country of origin before they meted out their racially-motivated ‘justice’). It would be easy to see this as simple racism, but I think we should also not overlook the impact of conspiracy theory and conspiracist dog-whistling in fomenting the rage, specifically the “Great Replacement” or “White Genocide” conspiracy theory.

The role of the Great Replacement conspiracy theory

According to the Great Replacement, white people are under threat in their native UK, due to the influx of immigrants from outside of Europe. This influx is, the conspiracists argue, no coincidence – it is deliberate, intentional, and coordinated. It will be no surprise as to who the conspiracists think is behind this concerted attack on the white British people: when the neo-Nazis marched with tiki torches in Charlottesville they chanted, “Jews will not replace us”. They weren’t afraid that their place would be taken by Jewish people; they feared that a shadowy cabal of influential Jews was trying to destabilise ‘native’ white countries by co-ordinating the mass influx of people of colour, who would soon replace the white race in their ‘own’ country.

Some Telegram message chats from 2021 discussing the 'kalergi plan' including a message which reads "they'll realise when the country is overrun with terrorists" and another which reads "definitely, we know how people are now after this Covid debacle, easily fooled, they still be saying these poor Afghans while they're chasing them down the street with a machete".
References to the Kalergi Great Replacement plan in Liverpool-based conspiracy groups, as spotted and reported in 2021. Note the warning about refugees ‘chasing them down the street with a machete’ – an uncanny portent to the fake news which kicked off the Southport riot.

Explicit references to the Great Replacement, or “The Kalergi Plan”, or themes central to the conspiracy theory are near-constant in the conspiracist Telegram groups which started during the pandemic and have continued to spread scaremongering misinformation since. What’s more, those explicit references have been augmented by high-profile dog whistling – Nigel Farage’s “Breaking Point” poster (among many other examples from the current MP for Clacton), David Cameron warning of a “Swarm” of migrants, Suella Braverman claiming we are experiencing an “invasion”, Rishi Sunak creating the “Stop the Boats” slogan which echoed around the riots, even David Davies warning that asylum seekers might be lying about their age in order to enter the country.

The trope that most refugees are “fighting age men” is constantly brought up as a gotcha, despite there being good reason why men are more likely to seek refuge before their family. It’s also deliberate that they’re described as of “fighting age” rather than “working age”, or simply “adult”. The effect is to create and stoke the fear that refugees are here to fight, with the inevitable implication being that the white people of the UK must fight back, or lose ‘our’ land.

Some Telegram message chats from 2021 with messages discussing white nationalism. One photo is a blonde woman with her hair in a ponytail on one knee with her head bowed. She is wearing a suit of armour and holding a sword. The text over the top reads "#WWG1WGA" which stands for 'where we go one we go all' and is a phrase commonly used by people who believe in the QAnon conspiracy theory.
White Christian Nationalist posts in Covid conspiracy Telegram groups, as spotted and reported in 2021. The Christian Knight post on the left was re-shared subsequently in the wake of the riots.

Arguably, nobody is more central to the point at which mainstream dog whistles met with closed-group extremism than Stephen Yaxley Lennon, aka “Tommy Robinson”.

Tommy Robinson

Back when I was investigating the White Rose antivax movement on Telegram, I warned that those conspiracy spaces I was monitoring were being constantly filled by antivax content originating with Tommy Robinson’s channel. To the point where, when I was talking to our friend Lewi, who was the mod of some of those channels, I even asked why Tommy Robinson’s content appeared so frequently, and he admitted that he needed to get better sources, but it was hard to find things elsewhere that had the kind of antivax messages Tommy Robinson’s channel was reliably pumping out.

Whenever I give talks about the antivax movement, I’ve pointed this out, that Tommy Robinson would prolifically post dozens of times per day, antivax and conspiracist posts, that would go viral across Telegram. And either it was because when the pandemic came along, Tommy and his crew became instantly radicalised into antivaxxers… or, they simply recognised that there were a lot of confused, scared and disaffected people during the pandemic, and if you pivoted to producing the kind of content they liked, plenty of them would follow you. Sure enough, Tommy Robinson started singing their song, and they started buying his album – his channel amassed hundreds of thousands of followers. And once they were following, he could start to feed them the kind of messaging he wanted them to believe – about Islam, about immigrants, and about England.

It is likely that the riots would not have happened were it not for Tommy Robinson. In the week before the killings, he held a rally in London and attracted 30,000 people – where he screened a film in which he falsely accused a Syrian teenager Jamal Hijazi of a violent assault, resulting in Hijazi receiving death threats. Robinson was successfully sued by Hijazi for defamation and owed £100,000 in damages. He didn’t pay. Instead, he repeated the claims and then fled the country to avoid the law.

A post from @TRobinsonNewEra which reads "ask yourselves do you want to become a minority to Islam in your own country & what does that look like"

He has retweeted a post which says "Demographic changes in India, Pakistan and Bangladesh. 1950 Pakistan ~20% Hindus (1.5% today) ... show more" - included in the tweet is a colour coded map of India and Pakistan.

Robinson was in a luxury hotel in Cyprus when, a few days later, the murders happened in Southport. He used his Twitter to amplify the misinformation about the killer, pointing almost a million followers in the direction of the riots. His Telegram channel shared the times and places of other planned riots and shared a hit list of immigration offices to target. On August 2nd, he appeared on Info Wars, where he told Alex Jones that “Between now and October, I’m going to pour petrol on the fire.” Whether that use of imagery was intentional or not, two days later, his followers tried to burn down two hotels filled with asylum seekers, while chanting his name.

Elsewhere, he has used his Twitter account to warn that white people are going to become a minority in the UK, and to release a video in which he further incited violence, saying:

“Stop the fucking boats. Get them out of them hotels. Get them gone. Put them on boats and send them back. They shouldn’t be here. They’re endangering the safety of our families. Men will rise up – they were always going to rise up. They have to rise up to defend their families. You’ve brought this war on our shores (sic).”

A tweet from Tommy Robinson which reads "Keir Starmer just made an address where he labels everyone upset about the murder of 3 little girls by Axel Muganwa Rudakubana as "thugs" and gives police more powers to prevent protests, even ushering in facial recognition to do so!"

A reply to the tweet is from Elon Musk which reads "!!"

According to The Times, Robinson’s incendiary tweets from his luxury hotel were seen more than fifty million times per day over the course of the riots. This despite him having previously been banned from Twitter for hate speech – a decision which was overturned when Elon Musk intervened in November. Those looking to Twitter’s ownership to curb these flagrant attempts to rile up a mass following and set them upon an innocent population will be sorely disappointed –Musk appears to view Tommy Robinson as less of a threat to stable democracy, and more of a reputable news source, even responding directly to Robinson’s propagandist mischaracterisations of events.

Musk’s inability to distinguish right-wing agitators from legitimate sources of news is incredibly concerning. We’ve seen him amplifying a post from propagandist Andy Ngo about William Nelson Morgan, a rioter who was convicted “for refusing to disperse and holding a stick at a library riot in Walton West Yorkshire”. “Messed up”, commented Musk. For what it’s worth, the man in question was holding a truncheon and was threatening people with it, and required three police officers to restrain him. Also for what it’s worth, the library attack was the one in Walton, Liverpool – not Walton, the village in West Yorkshire. When one of the most powerful men in the country gets his crime reporting from someone who doesn’t even bother to find out where the crime happened, we should be concerned.

A tweet from Elon Must which reads "Messed up" as he quote tweets a post from Andy Ngô that reads "William Nelson Morgan, a 69-year-old retiree with no criminal history, is the oldest anti-mass migration English riot arrestee to be convicted so far. He was sentenced to two years and eight months in a prison for refusing to disperse and holding a stick at a library riot in Walton, West Yorkshire on Aug. 4."
Elon Musk shares Andy Ngo’s misleading summary of the arrest of William Nelson Morgan, including misidentifying the location of the crime (image has been edited to include the entirety of Ngo’s tweet)

After the dust settles

Since the riots last week, the dust has somewhat settled, and order appears to have been restored. As a nation, we’re left with a clean-up job – something that community members across the country got on with very quickly, with people quickly volunteering to replace smashed windows, rebuild damaged walls, and repair broken trusts.

But while the physical damage has been all but repaired, the social damage will take a lot longer, and it will start by trying to understand the forces that drove the riots, and that provoked people into violence and disorder.

It’s impossible to deny the amount to which racism was responsible, including racism from within those communities. Mosques were targeted for a reason, but more than that, people weren’t pausing to ask who was Muslim, Sikh, Christian, Hindu or atheist; people were attacked for the colour of their skin and their perceived differences. We saw people doing Nazi salutes on the street, and proudly showing off swastika tattoos. They were there because they dislike people who don’t look like them. Equally, as was evidenced by the looting of shops (including Lush and Greggs), and by the number of rioters who turned out to have long criminal records, some of the riots attracted people who wanted to be part of the chaos.

However, there were undeniably a large number of people who were drawn to the riots for reasons that were not explicitly or consciously malign – people who were confused, angry and scared, who saw some children had been senselessly attacked and killed, and who worried that theirs could be next. Their fear and confusion made them malleable, and easy for cynical actors to manipulate them into directing that anger at scapegoats and political targets. There are still people, now, who believe that the killer was Muslim, or was a refugee, even though it’s unequivocally the case that he was not.

That anger, confusion and fear has been built and stoked for years, behind the scenes, in private Telegram groups where people are trained to accept dubious sources, and rewarded for spreading unconfirmed conjecture. It has been amplified on Twitter, where extremists like Tommy Robinson’s path heads in the same direction as people like Nigel Farage, Darren Grimes, Douglas Murray, Isabelle Oakenshot, Andy Ngo, and a carnival of other ‘social commentators’ who willingly stoke the immigration culture war if it happens to gain them money and influence.

This isn’t to say that last week’s riots were inevitable. But in many ways, they were predictable, and the warning signs had been gathering for a while. These riots had nothing to do with the deaths of three young girls, or the wounding of six others, and two adults; they had everything to do with a country which has ignored the role of conspiracism in growing the far right, a government that has actively fanned the flames of that growing fire, and a culture of social media sites who would rather monetise the people pouring on petrol than to turn off the fuel at the source.

“But it’s just a joke!”: why comedy’s right to offend doesn’t include the right to harm

0

Keeping your epistemological toolkit up to date is vitally important, but amongst the meticulous intellectual minutiae, a pearl of wisdom from over half a century ago is frequently forgotten. Thankfully, there’s a song to help you remember it: in 1968 Joan Ganz Cooney and Lloyd Morrisett had a goal to create a television program to help young children, particularly those in deprived areas, prepare for school.

That program was Sesame Street and, on the very first episode, the kindergarten crop was invited to investigate the complex phenomenon of dissimilarity with the bona fide earworm “One of these things is not like the others”.

Considering the near ubiquitous nature of Sesame Street in the public consciousness, it’s disappointing that some of the simple lessons easily learned by children are forgotten by adults when discussing matters of concern. This may be deliberate in some cases, but not always, I suspect.

So, to facilitate a recap, let’s look for some divergent properties in the ongoing discourse around so-called ‘cancel culture’ and/or ‘consequence culture’ in comedy. Particularly relevant at the time of writing as Joe Rogan has taken a break from platforming pseudoscientists and conspiracy theorists on his podcast to release yet another comedy special on Netflix, where he reminds us how controversial he is and how many times he has been cancelled.

Mans-laughter

Can you imagine a comedian today getting away with jokes about domestic violence, race, sexual assault, the Holocaust, 9/11, the LGBTQ community, and a host of other taboos? Surely not possible in the current climate, yet Anthony Jeselnik not only exists, but his career continues to go from strength to strength. Later this year he will bring his comedic marmite to the UK, performing in 3,000-capacity theatres rather than the low-budget comedy clubs you might expect to be hosting someone so far over the line of political correctness.

There has been some controversy around some of his content, but in general there are no calls for a boycott, even though there’s a significant portion of his most recent special for Netflix dedicated to his proficiency in dropping babies. Despite all this, no noticeable pitchforks or flaming torches outside his shows. So, why is this one not like the others?

The answer is simple: it’s intention.

Jeselnik’s entire schtick is that he’s the bad guy. His villainy is so pronounced and absurd that it’s beyond pantomime, and his audience is clearly aware of that. Even the casual observer who happens upon his material can easily spot the fact that his jokes are so far past the boundaries of good taste that it has to be an act.

Brent out of shape

So, shouldn’t this apply to all comedians? Well, no. One of these things is not like the others.

Joe Rogan is only one of many out there lashing out at those who are crying foul at their content: let’s consider the current self-proclaimed Christ in the crucifixion crosshairs of cancel culture, Ricky Gervais.

Over the years he has built up his stand-up comedy reputation as a dropper of truth bombs, ridiculing the absurd, and taking those who (supposedly) deserve it down a peg or two. His ruthless assassinations of those around him in the film and television industry during the Golden Globes is viewed by many as a righteous skewering of powerful and successful people, and by others as somewhat mean spirited. Either way, he’s referring to real-life people, many of whom have made real-life mistakes.

There’s occasionally exaggeration, some level of artistic licence, but a significant part of the impact his comedy makes is because he appears to actually mean what he says.

The approach that a comedian takes to their content will therefore have an effect on the type of audience they may attract but, more importantly, on the beliefs and sentiments of those people. People walking out of an Anthony Jeselnik show will, for the most part, be saying “That was so wrong!”, whereas Gervais’ audience might be saying “That was so right!”. The latter would almost certainly bring some of that baggage home with them, and absorb it into their worldview.

Target audience

The principle of influencing people, hopefully for the better, or speaking truth to power using comedy isn’t a new one. As someone who grew up in the 80s and 90s I feasted on politically motivated comedy from the likes of Ben Elton, Mark Thomas, and television programs such as Spitting Image, doing their best to highlight the harms done to the UK (and beyond) under the Prime Ministerial reign of Margaret Thatcher.

Margaret Thatcher's caricature puppet from the British satirical TV show, Spitting Image
The Spitting Image pupper of Margaret Thatcher. By mattbuck, CC BY-SA 2.0, via Wikimedia Commons

In more recent times we’ve seen the likes of Ricky Gervais target organised religion, and Dave Chappelle being a constant thorn in the side of American white supremacy. Those are seen as fair targets, so it’s particularly disappointing that both Gervais and Chappelle have recently turned their attention to much more marginalised demographics. Once again, one of these things is not like the others.

Gervais goes a little way to show his commitment to environmentalism by recycling the seemingly perpetual “I identify as [object]” attempted joke in his latest special. Credit where credit’s due though, he at least tries to approach it from a slightly different angle, unlike Joe Rogan who haplessly hammers his way through some of the most brainless stereotypes about trans people with all the subtlety of, and a fair amount of physical similarity to, Ram Man from Masters of the Universe.

Gervais isn’t just satisfied with transphobia though, as he takes some time to have a pop at refugees as well. While making keen observations to the howling crowd about why those risking their lives to get to the UK are majority male, he conveniently fails to elucidate on some of the heartbreaking reasons that this is the case.

This is commonly known as punching down instead of punching up, a phrase that was misconstrued badly by Jimmy Carr in an interview with Conan O’Brien, where his response to such accusations was “So you think there’s people below me?”. Carr is intelligent enough to be aware of the existence of metaphors, and he leans much more in the direction of the Jeselnik camp than the Gervais camp in terms of how seriously he takes his own jokes, so his misinterpretation and deflection seems strange, particularly considering he’d already spoken about the relevance of intent earlier in the interview.

To make it clear, when someone says “punching down”, it usually means that the butt of the joke may be the target of some form of vilification or discrimination in much more serious ways. It’s disappointing to have to spell it out, but at least we have Sesame Street to help with our spelling.

Harm offensive

In his latest special for Netflix, Gervais takes a pop at his detractors. “You can’t say that!” he says in a whiny voice, mimicking those who have complained about his content. He then turns to look at his audience, returns to his regular voice and coldly proclaims “I just did”. The crowd goes wild. Rapturous applause and cheers ring out through the massive auditorium. No laughter, but that’s not important right now. The shared glowing warmth of imagined righteousness is all that matters here.

This straw manning of valid criticisms of content is commonplace. Frequently you will hear comedians, or their fans, claiming that the baying mob is describing their content as “offensive”, and then going on to (sometimes correctly) say that the right to offend is an important part of any society that values free speech. Right enough, some people do use the word “offensive”, but not all. It gives the comedians in question justification enough to dismiss all the criticism coming in their direction, but one of these things is not like the others.

What is seemingly ignored is that many of the complaints come from people who are saying that the content is harmful rather than offensive. It could be both, of course, but it’s the harmful nature of some of the rhetoric that deserves attention.

It’s no secret that there’s an overtly hateful and dehumanising attitude towards immigrants and refugees in a disturbingly large percentage of the UK’s population. Look no further than Reform UK’s brazen “Stop the boats” rhetoric for evidence of that sentiment.

So, when Ricky Gervais takes an ignorant potshot at refugees, he’s not just being offensive, he’s fanning the flames of intolerance. Of course, maybe he could really just be joking, and being misunderstood by some of the less pleasant members of our society, in a similar way to Alf Garnett or Al Murray’s Pub Landlord character. It seems strange that no one has asked him about this during interviews.

Al Murray’s comedy character, the Pub Landlord. By Isabelle Adams, CC BY-SA 3.0, via Wikimedia Commons.

Gag reflex

This brings us on to the final defence of the ‘offensive’ comedian: the timeless classic, “It’s just a joke”.

Joe Rogan is quite the master of this art, setting himself up as the comedy goofball as he compares gay men to mountain lions, and reminds his audience that they shouldn’t be getting their medical information from him, forgetting that it’s frequently the people he invites on to his podcast and allows to spout misinformation that people are listening to (one of these things isn’t like the other, Joe!).

The best way to wave off any criticism without any kind of introspection is just to remind people you’re a comedian and you shouldn’t be taken seriously, except when you should.

YouTube comments are awash with people, presumably in a more advanced age bracket, complaining that people can’t take a joke anymore like they could in the ‘good old days’, and if you don’t like it then you should just move along. Let’s kick the tyres of that argument with some examples. See if you can spot which one of these things is not like the others:

Joke 1:

I recently commented on the choice of actors to appear in a movie about ancient Iran.
- Cast as Persians?
No, I was very complimentary.

Joke 2:

What's the best dressed geometrical shape?
- Apparellelogram.

Joke 3:

I've heard that the former Minister of State for Brexit Opportunities is now focusing his attention on rural air pollution.
- That awful country smog?
Yeah, that's him.

All three jokes are forms of torturous wordplay penned by me (yes, I’m an absolute joy to follow on social media), but the last of them alludes to a left-leaning political stance, and it has a specific human ‘target’. So, it’s pretty easy to see that a joke might not just be a joke. Those who are subjected to the joke may be influenced by it and may make a judgment on the values and ideals of the teller of the joke because of it.

The teller of any joke should consider this carefully, particularly if they have a massive global audience. For a beautiful illustration of this, check out Stewart Lee’s dismantling of the ‘classic’ lineup of Top Gear.

Can sell culture

So, we can see that comedy is always a complex landscape. Many performers skate the lines of good taste, and push boundaries. Some have a confusing mixture of good and bad intentions and it’s hard to tease them apart at times.

In terms of audiences, every individual has their own boundaries in terms of what they find palatable, both in terms of content, character, values, and the personal behaviour of the performer in question (Louis CK comes to mind particularly, in terms of conduct). They will vote with their feet accordingly. In certain circumstances they may be compelled to encourage other people to do the same, and at the extreme they may ask the same of venues and streaming platforms. They have the freedom to do so.

It seems though that those who consistently draw negative attention for various reasons somehow manage to continue with their careers. Rogan, Gervais and Chappelle continue to release specials on massive streaming platforms and play to massive audiences wherever they travel. Louis CK bafflingly still has a career.

Scroll your way through the stand-up comedy of any streaming platform and you’ll see a plethora of people with provocative show titles to illustrate just how much they’ve been cancelled, or how offensive they are. Some appear to thrive on the controversy. There may of course be some cases out there where a career has been fatally harmed by the so-called joke police, but as we have seen again and again, one of these things is not like the others.

From the archives: Hypnosis and the Occult

0

This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 4, Issue 1, from 1990.

Please note, as an archive article, some of the positions espoused here have been superseded by the evidence, and the piece is reproduced here as part our efforts to make available the history of the magazine. A follow-up piece, from the same issue, looked more closely and skeptically at the claims for the clinical use of hypnotherapy.

Many people, even with a quite educated background, still believe that hypnotism (or hypnosis as it is generally referred to nowadays) has something to do with the occult. When some people hear that I practise hypnosis, and research into it, they give some such response as “What, you actually do this sort of thing and it works?”

When they say ‘this sort of thing’ they wave their fingers in the air with a stroking motion which indicates the confusion in their minds, and indicates the main sources from which they have derived their stereotype of hypnosis.

Hypnosis has got nothing whatsoever to do with the occult, with parapsychology, with spiritualism, or with the huge body of confused belief that James Randi has so appropriately characterised as ‘flimflam’. This, of course, will be hotly contested by many enthusiastic practitioners and believers in the latter, and they have their own folk-lore of how telepathy, psychokinesis and such-like magic wonders can be assisted by hypnotising their subjects.

Let me first examine the basis for this belief and why even some seemingly educated people wave their fingers in the air when hypnosis is mentioned. Towards the end of the 18th century, Anton Mesmer, a Viennese physician, came to Paris and opened salons where he and his followers practised what they called ‘animal magnetism’, an art that had a notable success with people suffering from various disorders, notably those of a hysterical and psychosomatic variety.

Mesmer’s thesis was not entirely original, but he brought it up to date by expressing it in the more scientific terms of the Enlightenment, which superseded the religious terms of a previous age. What he practised was very much like the exorcism of religious healers like Father Gassner who had preceded him. He maintained that there was a universal fluid that he called ‘animal magnetism’, with which he and other favoured cognoscenti were richly endowed, and that they could convey it to their patients (whose animal magnetism was deficient and deranged).

The waving of the fingers in the air relates to the supposed ‘magnetisation’ of patients by followers of Mesmer – mesmerists. The below illustration shows a 19th century artist’s impression of how a mesmerist operated, but it is quite inaccurate since the ‘passes’ were made either in light contact with the subject’s body, or very close. The stroking movement was after the model of stroking an iron bar with a steel magnet, and thus conveying the property of magnetism to the former from the latter.

Space does not permit the complexities of the career of Mesmer or an adequate account of the early mesmeric movement to be outlined here, but it must suffice to note that in the first half of the 19th century there was a flourishing mesmeric movement in most of the European countries and in the USA. This is dealt with in considerable historical detail by Ellenberger (1970) in his voluminous work, and in my own introductory text (Gibson, 1977) I try to trace the connections between mesmerism and occultism.

Mesmeric procedure certainly did have a beneficial effect on many sick people, and some eminent doctors, such as John Elliotson, embraced it. He introduced it into his practice at University College Hospital, an enthusiasm that eventually forced him to resign because of the opposition of the medical establishment. Almost any practice, however weird, will have a beneficial effect if it is applied by prestigeful doctors and believed in by trusting patients. This is the well-known placebo mechanism.

Good accounts of how mesmerism, in the later period, was actually applied, are given by Deleuze (1837) and by Esdaile (1852), and interested readers are referred to these original sources and warned that some contemporary writers are shockingly inaccurate in their descriptions of it.

But the mesmerists did not stop at trying to treat pain and disease. Many of them believed that they were possessed of miraculous power and were capable of producing all sorts of psychological and physical wonders. The mesmeric movement attracted every sort of crank, charlatan, and believer in the occult.

Using the sloppy sort of methods of inquiry that have always characterised the parapsychology movement, they convinced the gullible, and sometimes themselves, that they could teach people to see without the use of their eyes, transfer thoughts, prophesy the future and perform other forms of magic. Consequently, the pillars of orthodox medicine seized upon this all too blatant display of flim-flam, and refused to believe in some quite genuine cases in which severe surgical operations had been conducted painlessly.

When Dr Elliotson tried to present the case of a man whose leg had been painlessly amputated with the aid of mesmerism, many of his colleagues refused to listen, and one of them facetiously inquired if the man had yet been taught to read with the back of his neck, as this was a feat that other mesmerists had claimed.

But all sceptical doctors in the early 19th century were not so implacably unbelieving. Sir John Forbes (1845), although utterly contemptuous of the parapsychological aspect of the mesmeric movement and concerned to expose its charlatanry, gave a fair consideration to the considerable evidence that mesmerism was a genuine phenomenon and capable of producing an impressive degree of anaesthesia in certain circumstances. Testimony from such critics as Forbes is far more impressive than that from enthusiasts such as Elliotson.

Hypnotism is not mesmerism

In stating quite categorically that hypnotism should be distinguished from mesmerism I am aware that I differ in this from some quite eminent theorists. The difference is well put by Stam and Spanos (1982, pages 14-15) in the following passage:

the concept of hypnosis is, at best, vague and ambiguous and many of the behaviours associated with this label have changed quite substantially over the years… For most investigators the term ‘hypnosis’ refers to an ill-defined altered state of consciousness, the characteristics of which have never been clearly denoted or agreed upon… Moreover, there is little agreement as to variables that are necessary and sufficient for producing an hypnotic state. For historical reasons most investigators employ hypnotic inductions that consist primarily of verbal suggestions for relaxation and sleep… Some (Sheehan and Perry, 1976) argue that the largely non-verbal ‘passes’ made by Mesmer and his followers were variations of hypnotic induction procedures.

As I see it, we have much to gain in reserving the term mesmerism for the procedures that were fully described by the more literate of the 19th century mesmerists. It should be noted that these procedures relied on non-verbal manipulations, sometimes lasting for hours, and producing an unresponsive torpid state much resembling tonic immobility. By contrast, hypnotism is essentially a verbal procedure, the hypnotist suggesting ideas to the subject, and working upon the latter’s powers of imagination, to produce a state that may be very responsive and active, and , when well developed, has traditionally been called ‘somnambulism’.

We need not pursue the question of the differences between mesmerism and hypnotism any further here, except to observe that the two may exist coincidentally. An illustration in George Du Maurier’s famous novel, Trilby, shows Svengali making manual passes in the air in front of the girl, but he is also murmering “Maintenant, dors ma mignonne”, thus using a blend of mesmerism (inaccurately portrayed) and hypnotism – the verbal suggestion of sleep.

Some mesmerists undoubtedly used a measure of verbal suggestion, working on their subjects’ powers of suggestibility and fantasy, as well as immobilising them and lulling them with long-continued, rhythmic passes. What is evident, however, is that the evil reputation that was acquired by the mesmerists, a reputation for charlatanry and claiming occult powers, became attached to those who practised and investigated hypnotism from the earliest times.

Historically, the earliest hypnotist in the 19th century, in the sense of employing verbal suggestions to induce psychological changes, was the Abbe de Faria, whose book, published in 1819, rejected the whole mesmeric flim-flam of animal magnetism and sought to establish a sound psychological basis for his discoveries in hypnosis.

However it is to James Braid, a Scottish physician working in Manchester, that we owe the term ‘hypnotism’. At first he thought that the phenomena he induced were due to sheerly neurological mechanisms, and because of his ‘scientific’ stance and rejection of the mesmeric claims, he received a sympathetic hearing in at least some respectable medical circles. Later, he realised that really the hypnotic phenomena were due to psychological mechanisms, the suggestions of the hypnotist affecting a change in the state of consciousness of the subject.

His ideas were later taken up in France, and the patronage of such ideas by prestigious figures such as the great neurologist Charcot at the Salpetriere Hospital in Paris, and Bernheim at Nancy, made the topic of hypnosis relatively respectable in the medical and scientific world. But the mesmerists clung to their occult beliefs. At the International Conference on Magnetism in 1889 they emphasised that their master was the long-dead Mesmer, and that magnetism should not be confused with hypnotism.

Reasons for ultra-scepticism about hypnosis

When I was a lecturer in Psychology at Hatfield Polytechnic I continued my researches in hypnosis, researches I had begun in the 1950s at the Institute of Psychiatry. It was interesting to see the degree to which the public image of hypnosis had improved over the years and how the subject was gaining acceptance in orthodox Psychology.

Yet even so I still had fingers waved in the air by people who should have known better. At one time the rumour went round that I was conducting experiments in telepathy, and hence I had some difficulty in persuading a few students that I had no time for parapsychology malarky, and that I would not steal their minds away if they consented to be my subjects for hypnosis experiments.

It occurred to me, however, that in some quarters the misunderstanding was not wholly unintentional, for if you are engaged in boring experiments messing around with computers, or trying to get students to participate in dull experiments in psychophysics, a colleague engaged in highly interesting experiments in hypnosis excites envy. The students are only too willing to participate, so can it be wholly respectable? Does it not smell of the parapsychology game, ouija boards and table-turning?

It occurs to me that other psychologists have also encountered this difficulty. The great experimental psychologist Clark Hull made valuable advances in the investigation of hypnosis and suggestibility in the 1920s, yet when he obtained a new professorial chair at Yale, he had to give up his work in hypnosis research because of the extreme prejudice against the subject there. Hull wrote in 1933:

Such, in brief, is the history of hypnotism. All sciences alike have descended from magic and superstition, but none has been so slow as hypnosis in shaking off the evil associations of its origin.

Some practitioners of clinical hypnosis, of course, do not want to shake off the legacy of the past with all the associations with mesmerism and the occult , as they wish to appear as miracle-workers and mystery men. One such practitioner, who now has a huge and prosperous posthumous following in the ‘New Age’, was an American psychiatrist of the name of Milton Erickson. The late Dr Erickson rivalled the late Ron Hubbard in his pretensions, and followed the practice of Anton Mesmer in wearing purple, the colour of emperors and magicians (‘Principle 1. Wear lots of purple. All good Ericksonians know that it was Milton’s favourite colour because he was colour-blind except for purple. Also, all legitimate books on Erickson are bound in purple’, Chamberlain, 1988).

Many academic psychologists, anxious to pursue a respectable career in experimental psychology, have reacted and over-reacted to the supposed overtones of occultism in modern hypnosis, and leaned over backwards in their declared scepticism, so that many lay people are wondering whether the very topic of hypnosis is merely a con, and whether the concept should be relegated along with telepathy, psychic spoon-bending, dowsing, and a lot else, to the world of the flim-flam.

Thus Hearne (1982) argues that hypnosis is an umbrella term covering a whole range of known psychological phenomena, and that the concept has no scientific utility so the term should be abandoned. His main plank is that, in the psychological laboratory, by means of ‘task-motivating instructions’, we can get students to do the same things and report the same things without hypnosis as with hypnosis.

Another name for ‘task-motivating instructions’ is ‘intimidation’. This really relates to some old psychological experiments by Asch (1958) who showed that if you rigged the laboratory situation sufficiently you could compel many laboratory subjects to deny the evidence of their senses and report just about anything you wanted them to report, and the experiments of Milgram (1963) in which some fairly normal people were intimidated into doing some highly abnormal things in the supposed infliction of pain on others.

Such experimenters do not face the fact that people may say and do the same things for totally different reasons. A student may report that he felt no pain in a laboratory experiment either (a) because a negative hallucination for pain had been induced by hypnotic suggestion; or (b) because he’ll get in the professor’s bad books if he does admit that it hurt a bit, and thus spoil the professor’s hypothesis.

It should be noted that although Hearne, writing in 1982, supported his article by 15 published studies, none of them is later than 1967, with the exception of two articles by himself and with a colleague. His paper was old hat. The peak of this ultra-scepticism came at the end of the 1960s with the publication of a book by T.X. Barber (1969). Barber was, at that time, the Big Bad Wolf of the hypnosis world, the Ultra Skeptic No. 1. He has softened since, and twenty years later seems to admit that hypnosis is, after all, a useful concept.

But there have been many contenders for the mantle of Barber and the title of the Ultra Skeptic No. 1. Graham Wagstaff, an ex-collaborator of Hearne’s, published a book in which hypnosis was more or less equated with compliance, and in his book he cited T.X. Barber 213 times! Wagstaff’s book is interesting in that he presents his case like a very good barrister presenting a highly dubious case, yet seeking to implant in the minds of the jury sufficient suspicion that will eventually be developed into certainty that the accused was (or was not) guilty. With regard to compliance, early in the 1982 book, page 55, he writes:

In this chapter examples have been given to illustrate the importance of compliance in determining hypnotic behaviours. However, as the final study seems to indicate, there appears to be more to certain reports by some subjects than compliance; it appears that some subjects may genuinely believe themselves to be, or to have been, in a special condition or state of ‘hypnosis’.

Of course these subjects are mistaken about themselves, Wagstaff proceeds to show, for as he sees things, there is no such thing as a special condition or state of ‘hypnosis’. And having given this cautious caveat, he goes on throughout the book to demonstrate that all the phenomena of hypnosis could be explained in terms of compliance, and so the ultra-sceptical layman who thinks that the hypnotic subject is just doing as he is told, may well be right!

The latest contender for the mantle of Barber (1969) and the title of the Ultra Skeptic No. 1 is Nicholas Spanos at Carleton University. Spanos has even gone the length of devising his own scale for measuring hypnotic responsiveness – there were already quite a few well-known and widely used modern scales published, but they did not suit Spanos’ special requirements. And what are these requirements? Let me quote a published commentary on Spanos’ creation, the Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestions Scale (CURSS) from Kihlstrom, 1985, page 387; emphasis added*:

Recently, Spanos and his associates introduced the Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale (CURSS; Spanos et al 1983c,d)… While the CURSS clearly taps the domain of hypnosis to some degree, it also tends to define hypnosis in terms of the subject’s willingness to cooperate with the procedures rather than in terms of subjective experience, as is characteristic of the Stanford scales.

Enough said. Recently Spanos contributed an article to the Skeptical Inquirer (Spanos, 1987-88) in the course of which he set out his own theory of hypnosis and made what were, to my mind, certain misstatements of fact.

I therefore wrote to the SI pointing out that another writer had correctly identified at least seven other respected theories of hypnosis that are accepted by the scientific community, and that Spanos’ theory was not the only acceptable one, as readers might suppose. I also called attention to a misstatement of fact that he had written, and pointing out that the position that he takes “has finally been abandoned by most serious research scientists”.

My letter (Gibson, 1988) was published alongside a reply from Spanos, in the course of which he writes, “According to Gibson, this position has been abandoned ‘by most significant research scientists’. Unfortunately Gibson never tells us who these scientists are or what evidence led them to abandon this position.”

Unfortunately it would take up many pages of space to name all these scientists, and to cite all the published studies; indeed many significant scientists researching in the field of hypnosis have never held this position in the first place. However, it is up to me to let readers know just who the scientists are and where their arguments can be read in a concise form, if readers are interested. Fortunately, this task is easy. I would refer readers to the journal Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9, pp. 449-502; (1987) 10, pp. 519-529; pp. 773-76; (1988) 11, pp. 712-714.

Spanos writes the initial article, but this is followed in the same issue of the journal by no less than 22 contributions from other scientists, some of them highly critical of Spanos’ work, and charging him with failure to cite all the relevant evidence, and themselves providing many additional references relevant to the controversy that Spanos had not mentioned. The controversy continued in the 1987 and 1988 issues of the journal that I have mentioned, 12 more protagonists leaping into the fray!

The dangers of ultra-scepticism

I am a highly sceptical person myself, and concerned about all the rubbish, codswallop, half-truths and sheer lies about hypnosis that the public is being fed with, mostly by interested people who aim to make a living out of it by preserving the occult aura that has bedevilled it for so long.

Why then, should I object to the ultra-sceptics’ efforts to debunk it completely out of existence? As I see it, by leaning too far over backwards and reinforcing the view held by some laymen that ‘It’s all a con’, they do, in fact, play into the hands of those who are anti-science and gleefully proclaim that as scientists can’t agree, maybe the occultists were right and it’s all a question of mysterious fluids of animal magnetism.

I do not claim that we know all about hypnosis; I have been researching in the area for thirty years and there are still mysteries about it that still amaze me, but I also know that considerable progress has been made by applying the methods of natural science in its investigation, and I am optimistic enough to expect that we shall continue to make progress.

Let me end by quoting the great ex-Ultra Skeptic No. 1, T.X. Barber, who ended his 1969 book thus on page 242:

Although research in ‘hypnosis’ promises to provide a broader understanding of human behavior, the reverse is also true – as psychologists working on other topics develop general principles, their principles should help us attain a deeper understanding of the topic ‘hypnosis’. Finally, the topic ‘hypnosis’ may lose all of its aura, mystery, and separate status and become integrated into general psychology.

I agree with him there.

References

  • Asch, S.E. (1958), Effects of group pressure upon modification and distortion of judgements. In E.E. Maccoby & E.L. Hartley (Eds.) Readings in Social Psychology, (3rd Edition) New York: Halt, Reinhart & Winston.
  • Barber, T.X. (1969), Hypnosis: A Scientific Approach. New York: Van Nostrand Reinholt Co.
  • Chamberlain, L. (1988), How to be an Ericksonian (Milton not Erik). Swedish J. Hypnosis, 15, 128-130.
  • Carli, G. (1978), Animal hypnosis and pain. In F .H. Frankel & H.S. Zamanski (Eds.) Hypnosis at Its Bicentennial, New York: Plenum Press.
  • Deleuze, J.P.F. (1937), Practical Instructions in Animal Magnetism, (Trans. T.S. Harshorn) Providence R.I.
  • B. Cranston & Co. Ellenberger, H.F. (1970), The Discovery of the Unconscious. New York: Basic Books.
  • Elliotson, J. (1843), Numerous Cases of Surgical Operations Without Pain in the Mesmeric State, London
  • H. Bailliere. Esdaile, J. (1952), The Introduction of Mesmerism as an anaesthetic and Curative Agent into the Hospitals of India, Perth
  • Dewar & Sons. Forbes, J. {1845), Mesmerism True – Mesmerism False, London: J. Churchill. Gibson, H.B. (1977), Hypnosis: Its Nature and Therapeutic Uses, London
  • Peter Owen. Gibson, H.B. (1988), Understanding hypnosis. Skeptical Inquirer, 13, pp. 106-107.
  • Hearne, K. (1982), A cool look at nothing special. Nursing Mirror, 20 January Hull, C.L. (1933), Hypnosis and Suggestibility: An Experimental Approach, New York
  • Appleton Century Crofts. Kihlstrom, J.F. (1985), Hypnosis. Ann. Rev. Psychol., 36, pp. 385-418
  • Melzack, R. & Wall, P. {1966), Pain mechanisms: a new theory, Science, 150, pp. 971-979.
  • Milgram, S. (1963), Behavioral study of obedience. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol., 67, 371-378.
  • Spanos, N.P. (1987-88), Past-life hypnotic regression: A critical review. Skeptical Inquirer, 22, 174-180.
  • Stam, H.J. & Spanos, N.P. (1982), The Asclepian dream healings and hypnosis: a critique. Int. J. Clin. Exper. Hypnosis, 30, pp. 9-22.
  • Wagstaff, G.P. (1982), Hypnosis, Compliance, and Belief, Brighton: The Harvester Press.

*Note: emphasis added to this quote seems to have been lost in converting the archive article to its digital format.

We shouldn’t fear a “zombie fungus”, like The Last of Us… but a threat from fungi is real

The Last of Us has become one of the most popular and widely discussed TV series of the last two years. Its post-apocalyptic horror and references to a pandemic event turned up just at the right moment, when the world was starting to recover from the Covid-19 pandemic, and faced new crisis events. But, along with this fictional component, its scientific aspect has also attracted attention: it was the first time “zombie fungi” penetrated into popular culture so deeply.

The main fungal character of this series is a Cordyceps fungus. In real life, fungi of this genus can alter the behaviour of infected insects, turning them into a kind of ‘zombie’ whose only purpose is to ensure the fungus’ spread. The show’s thrilling plot raises a question: could such a scenario happen to humans? Is there a possibility that one day, a fungus emerges that will transform the infected into aggressive monsters?

More than a year ago, The Skeptic featured an article by Natália Pasternak, a microbiologist from Columbia University. Natália argues that the ‘zombie fungus’ threat is only fictional – ‘zombie fungi’ in insects are highly specialised and interact with the host’s brain via an intricate set of chemicals. They are highly unlikely to jump to humans.

The neurochemical mechanism of the ‘zombie fungus’ effect in an infected ant. There is no evidence that any fungus can trigger the same changes in any vertebrate including humans. Image credit: Lenapcrd/Wikimedia Commons/CC BY-SA 4.0. 

I fully agree with Natália, and could add that the phenomenon of behaviour-altering parasites is well characterised today. In humans, strong behaviour-altering action is exhibited by the rabies virus only. This virus makes its host aggressive and prone to bite, thus facilitating the spread of the virus through infected saliva. This is not a ‘zombie virus’, but our civilisation had gotten acquainted with it far earlier than we learned the word ‘zombie’. Behaviour-altering action is also suggested for Toxoplasma gondii – but, while it is well evidenced in mice, its effects are questionable in humans.

There is no evidence that any fungus can exert behaviour-altering action on humans. Moreover, there is no evidence for such an action in any vertebrate. Thus, I agree that the threat of being infected with a behaviour-altering fungus is negligible. But there is a real, conceivable danger to face new, emerging infections caused by fungi.

A more realistic fungal threat

Sixty five million years ago, a giant asteroid struck Earth, causing an effect comparable to a full-scale nuclear war. All large plants died at once due to an extreme blast of air and fire, and the planet was plunged into darkness by soot particles in the atmosphere.

In such harsh conditions, the biosphere turned into a “fungal compost”, says Dr Arturo Casadevall, infectious disease researcher. According to Dr Casadevall, this environment full of fungi made our small ancestors become warm-blooded. Most fungi are psychrophilic – this means they thrive in cool environments in relatively low temperatures, up to 30°C. Our body temperature of around 37°C grants us a narrow temperature barrier, just several degrees wide – that is unfavourable for most fungi. But, due to our warming climate, there is risk that things may change.

Fungi evolve rapidly and can easily adapt to observable climate change. The slow, degree-by-degree rise of global temperatures can shift their optimum temperature upwards – closer and closer to body temperature. If we lose our thermal barrier, we could be exposed to risks from a plethora of fungi that were previously innocuous to us.

This is not just theoretical speculation. The recent review in The Lancet Microbe cites some examples of fungal pathogens that have already shifted to humans from other habitats. The yeast Candida orthopsilosis, a human opportunistic pathogen, originated from a warm marine ecosystem. Fusarium oxysporum was a banana pathogen, but now it is becoming capable of causing infections in a number of human organs too, from skin to bones. Finally, the clinical strains of Candida auris – an emerging nosocomial pathogen – grow at higher temperatures than environmental strains.

A strain of Candida auris – an emerging fungal pathogen – cultured in a petri dish at a CDC laboratory. Its rise could be a consequence of climate change. Image credit: CDC/Wikimedia Commons/Public Domain.

It is possible that we are already facing the loss of our temperature barrier – and it is difficult to estimate which fungus is the next candidate to meet us. As a recent headline on Euronews pithily put it, our worst case scenario might be “The Last of Us minus the zombie part”.

The article by Natália Pasternak did mention these threats – but I feel compelled to put an additional emphasis on the real fungal threat. We should not be too calm. Instead of the fears of zombie disease, The Last of Us should be a promotion of knowledge about emerging fungal infections that are unfortunately underestimated, in contrast to viruses, for example.

What can we do? We need a system of tracking emerging and cross-kingdom fungal infections worldwide, to help us estimate and assess the danger. However, even that could be too late to stop the most detrimental events we might see unfold, since climate change cannot be reversed at once. This is one more reason to promote, invent and implement sustainable technologies right now.

Carbon emissions should become the problem for each of us – otherwise they will become the problem for the last of us. 

The 2024 UK General Election came with lessons that skeptics should listen to

Note: this article was written in late July 2024, the week prior to race riots powered by misinformation that swept across England and Belfast in Northern Ireland.

Pseudoscientific, conspiratorial, religious and overtly non-reality-based policies have always crept in on the edges of UK politics. In the 1990s, the Natural Law Party delighted TV viewers – if not voters – with party political broadcasts highlighting their belief that transcendental meditation and yogic flying would end poverty and bring about world peace, before ultimately disbanding in 2001.

Jump forward to the 2024 elections and the Scottish Family Party stood on a rather different religiously-motivated platform, based on “Judeo-Christian-inspired values.” As such, their 2023 policy document opposes same-sex marriage for the sake of children, which is of course completely contrary to the repeated, worldwide evidence. Scottish voters reassuringly and overwhelmingly rejected the party’s candidates.

Towards the mainstream, though, parties mostly seek to distance themselves from such fringe beliefs, with the Green Party’s growing aspirations clearly highlighted by their leadership moving away from – for example – support for homeopathy. While vestiges of those beliefs remain, such as the quickly-amended policy on natural childbirth, the speed with which it was amended this year shows that there is clearly risk in being associated with “woo-inclined” policies, and highlights a divide in the Greens between the old guard and younger, more evidence-focused supporters.

In fairness, not all such “woo-inclined” thinking is confined to the Greens, or even the left. MPs from across the political spectrum, from Labour and Conservative to SNP and Liberal Democrat, have signed Early Day Motions supporting homeopathy, although thankfully all these EDMs were from 2010 or earlier. An optimist might speculate that as the evidence on homeopathy has become more well-known, so MP behaviour has adjusted accordingly.

One political party in 2024 bucked the mainstream tendency towards following the evidence. Right-wing populist party Reform UK, which received the third largest number of votes (but came a distant joint-sixth in terms of constituencies won), embraced many policies in direct contrast to the other widely supported parties.

For example, on climate change, deputy leader Richard Tice said:

Net zero will make zero difference to climate change… The idea that you can stop the power of the sun or volcanoes is simply ludicrous.

The science, of course, says that humans, not the sun or volcanoes, are responsible for climate change. While the majority of UK voters do believe that climate change is real and requires action, there remains a persistent core of people more likely to vote for a party advocating the opposite. It is hard to see what can be done to change minds on this, considering the extremely high visibility and awareness around this issue.

Or to take a “culture war” issue, Reform UK’s manifesto promises to “scrap Diversity, Equality and Inclusion (DE&I) rules that have lowered standards and reduced economic productivity.” Again there is a sizeable minority of British people who think that EDI initiatives lead to unfair outcomes, and to whom Reform UK can appeal. This is one area where evidence and self-interest can meet: diversity in corporations, contrary to the Reform UK claims, correlates very clearly with economic benefits and improved financial performance. EDI isn’t just the right thing to do; it is the profitable thing to do. In the continuing cost of living crisis and economic underperformance of the UK, this may resonate with at least some anti-EDI hold-outs.

Beyond the manifesto, party leader Nigel Farage posted the following on social media:

Reform UK will reject the influence of the World Economic Forum and cancel Britain’s membership of it.

Aside from being nonsensical – no country is a member of the WEF – this kind of message directly reaches a certain group of conspiracy-minded folk who see the WEF and Klaus Schwab as being behind every conspiracy around, from Covid vaccinations and the Great Reset to 15 minute cities and eating insects for protein. Whether or not Reform UK intended to reach these audiences, they clearly did, with Sky News reporting that conspiracist groups on Telegram “posted 5,239 messages about Mr Farage/Reform UK – more than any other party”, and that:

One of the most prominent groups that focuses on the QAnon conspiracy theory… said it would be launching a Reform UK based group on Facebook with QAnon content in order to “bring a lot of traffic to the group”.

Al Baker, managing director of Prose, which analysed the data from Telegram, notes that while being a Reform UK voter doesn’t mean you are a conspiracy theorist, if you are a conspiracy theorist then “you are far more likely to support Reform UK than other parties.”

For some on the fringe, though, Reform UK does not go far enough. In The Light Paper and similar channels on Telegram, arguments erupted during the election campaign and its aftermath as to whether Reform UK are “controlled opposition”, if a vote for Reform UK was participating in a “psyop” and giving legitimacy to “globalist” Labour by boosting turnout, and whether Nigel Farage and Richard Tice are really out to fight the system – or just in it for themselves…

A hex-map of the UK's 2024 general election results by seat, showing largely red from Labour's significant margin of victory
A hex-style map showing the result of the 2024 UK general election, with Reform UK’s five seats in the east of England visible in turquoise. By Gust Justice, CC BY-SA 4.0, via Wikimedia Commons

As Al Baker notes, however, many Reform UK voters are likely unaware that they voted for the conspiracy theorists’ party of choice. Most Reform UK leaflets, after all, focused on their core policy messages around immigration and small boats, NHS waiting lists, taxes and energy bills. Most wider media coverage of Reform UK is, of course, based on the ample time given to Nigel Farage’s views by broadcasters and print alike.

Of the largest parties, Reform UK voters were least likely to vote tactically, with 85% of their voters saying they are making a positive choice to vote for Reform. This is unusually high compared to most parties’ voters in the UK. Whether motivated by conspiracy theories and culture-war grudges, or anger about the cost of living, immigration and the NHS, these voters aren’t going away. The systemic unfairness of the UK’s first-past-the-post voting system can’t be guaranteed to keep them out forever.

How to reach some of the one in seven voters who are enthusiastic about a party like Reform UK is something we should all take seriously.

The 2024 Olympics ceremony raises the spectre of pagan influence on the origins of Christianity

Since 1789, the French have taken pride in laïcité. This is the separation of religion from public life and state institutions, emphasising the neutrality of the state in religious matters and the freedom of individuals to practise any religion or none at all. France upholds strong protections for freedom of expression, even when it involves blasphemy, criticism or satire of religion.

A huge challenge for France in recent times has been the assimilation of migrants from Muslim countries – where secularism is very weak – while at the same time upholding the principles of laïcité. There is some truth in the claim that, in the name of anti-racism and multiculturalism, some political actors in the left side of the political spectrum throughout Europe have been too complacent with migrants who are not willing to embrace secularism. France has courageously stood against that complacency, as in the staunch defense of Charlie Hebdo after the attacks by terrorists offended by blasphemous cartoons.

But in the context of the rise of the nationalist far-right in France, a bigger challenge now ensues. For it is no longer only Muslim citizens who are quick to be offended by seemingly blasphemous symbols. Christians – who, ever since revolutionary times in France, had learned to accept that blasphemy cannot be considered a crime – also want a slice of the religious pie, setting back the clock to the Ancien régime.

The inauguration of the 2024 Paris Olympics is a case in point. The ceremony featured choreography that, to many, seemed like a blasphemous parody of Leonardo da Vinci’s The Last Supper. The backlash from many conservatives was intense. Influential Catholic bishop Robert Barron lamented the affair:

I see this clear mockery of the Last Supper, and for Christians, the Last Supper, when Jesus, in anticipation of his death, gives his body and blood to the world… it’s… at the center of Christianity, and to see… drag queens and so on, cavorting in imitation of da Vinci’s Last Supper, how could Christians not construe that as a slap?

Except that it was not – at least not this time, anyway. As the organisers of the event have now clarified, the dance was not inspired by The Last Supper, but rather by scenes from Greek mythology. Thomas Jolly – the artistic director – explained that the ceremony was inspired by Jan van Bijlert’s painting The Feast of the Gods, with a focus on the Greek god Dionysus. The rushed reaction of conservative Christians has been taken as evidence of their ignorance of the history of art.

I beg to differ. In my view, the ceremony could be plausibly interpreted either way. But this is not fortuitous. There are indeed resemblances between the theme of the Last Supper and feasts of pagan gods. In both religious narratives, some divine being eats a meal in the company of others, and it is therefore inevitable that, in the artistic depictions of both traditions, some resemblances might come out. Indeed, this raises a long-discussed issue in the history of religions: to what extent is Christianity based on pagan traditions?

The Feast of the Gods classical painting by Jan van Bijlert, featuring cavorting gods sat at a table with a white tableclothe and cherubs watching from behind the clouds
The Feast of the Gods, classical painting, Jan van Bijlert, CC0, via Wikimedia Commons

My view is that it is impossible to deny some pagan influence. Some authors have overblown the case and have argued that there was never a historical Jesus, but rather, the character portrayed in the New Testament is a fictional reinterpretation of pagan Mediterranean deities. This claim has been debunked many times by competent historians. But even granting that there was a historical Jesus and that some – but by no means all – stories narrated in the gospels did take place, it is only sensible to admit that the Christian interpretation of Jesus has been coloured by pagan influences.

Consider, for example, the halo. In many artistic depictions of Jesus – although not in da Vinci’s Last Supper – Jesus is presented with a halo. In the 2024 Olympics ceremony, the central figure – a large woman wearing a blue outfit – also sports a feature over her head that could be construed as a halo. Consequently, it is understandable that Christians might consider this a parody of Jesus. But, in Jan van Bijlert’s The Feast of the Gods, the god with a halo and occupying a central place is Apollo. In Roman and Greek cultures, halos were used to denote divine or exceptional figures. The halo represents the sun and, consequently, solar deities – Apollo being one of them – were typically adorned with halos.

Some authors who claim Jesus never existed suggest that the fictional character may have been modeled on solar deities. Again, this is a very questionable claim. In the New Testament, whatever associations are made between Jesus and the sun are tenuous at best and, in any case, Jesus is never described as having a halo.

Prior to the fifth century, halos were absent in Christian art. But by the early Middle Ages, depictions of Jesus began to incorporate the halo, and it is impossible to avoid the conclusion that this artistic feature carried pagan influences, although admittedly such interpretations of Jesus as a solar deity were not grounded in core Christian doctrine or tradition.

An ancient carved relief in stone showing the god Mithra, his head surrounded by lines denoting a solar halo
The God Mithra on the rock relief of Shapour II at Taq-e Bustan. Image by dynamosquito from France, CC BY-SA 2.0, via Wikimedia Commons

However, the theme of the Last Supper is at the core of Christianity, and here some pagan influence is notorious. As per traditional Christian interpretation, the occasion for the Last Supper was the Jewish Passover. Christians may object to the claim that their religion is grounded in pagan elements, but they acknowledge that Jesus was a Jew, and that Christianity derives from Judaism. In that regard, they are not at all bothered by the claim that the Last Supper has Jewish origins because, after all, the Last Supper was a Passover meal.

Yet, this is historically dubious. The gospels do not even agree on this point. The synoptic gospels – Matthew, Mark and Luke – claim that, indeed, the Last Supper was a Passover meal. But the gospel of John asserts that the Last Supper took place one day before Passover.

Historians do not usually lend much credibility to John’s version of events, but in this case, it seems that the Last Supper was not a Passover meal. The central elements of the Passover as per Jewish tradition are absent in the accounts of the synoptic gospels. There is no paschal lamb, no bitter herbs, no four cups of wine. Likewise, the book of Exodus suggest that Jews were not allowed to go out of their houses after the Passover but, as per the synoptics’ account, Jesus does so when he goes out to pray in the garden of Gethsemane.

If not a Passover celebration, what then was the Last Supper? If one accepts that Jesus anticipated that he would soon be arrested – a debatable premise – then it was probably a farewell meal. If, on the contrary, one believes that Jesus was completely surprised by his arrest (I personally lean more towards this hypothesis) then it was merely a regular meal. Either way, it seems very unlikely that during that meal, Jesus – a Jew first and foremost – would institute the ritual of the eucharist. Judaism has strong dietary regulations, and the idea of reenacting some form of cannibalism – even if only symbolically – would have been extremely alien to Jesus and his disciples.

Whence the eucharist, then? Many secular historians think the apostle Paul is the originator of this ritual and its theological implications. The ritual is mentioned for the first time in one of his letters – 1 Corinthians, which was written decades before the gospels. Paul was also a Jew but, unlike Jesus, he was Hellenistic. Consequently, Paul was much more familiar with Greek and Roman religious ideas that were widespread throughout the Mediterranean basin.

In that world, mystery religions were prevalent; one feature of such religions was the ritual consumption of an animal symbolising a deity in order to absorb its powers, particularly related to the idea of death and rebirth. It is not a long stretch of the imagination to assert that Paul incorporated these pagan ideas and practices into his interpretation of who Jesus was and, decades later, the gospels – all written by authors influenced by Paul’s ideas – developed the notion into a foundational narrative.

Not much is known about mystery religions, but it is plausible to think that their rituals involving meals were partly built on Greek mythological themes of banquets and feasts where gods dine on ambrosia, nectar and wine. Consequently, there may very well be some connection between pagan scenes of gods celebrating a big meal – such as the one depicted by Jan van Bijlert – and the Christian Last Supper – such as the one depicted by da Vinci. When a troupe of dancers ambiguously represents the former, it is not completely unexpected that many Christians would think that the dance is parodying the latter.

This affair should leave a couple of lessons. First, if European governments are concerned that some Muslim migrants may not be sufficiently willing to assimilate to the secular requirements of society, then Christians must lead with example and resist the urge to call for the criminalisation of blasphemy. Admittedly, in the aftermath of the Olympics ceremony, nobody called for beheadings or arrests, but the indignation was intense and Christians must make sure that this indignation never delves into requests for blasphemy laws.

Second, Christians should deepen their comprehension of their own religion’s history. This entails coming to terms with the fact that Christianity did borrow some elements from pagan cults and other religions. In so doing, Christians will be in a better position to understand that particular artistic expressions that may at first seem a blasphemous attack against their traditions are nothing of the sort.